The Stanford marshmallow experiment was a series
of studies on delayed
gratification in the late 1960s and early
1970s led by psychologist Walter Mischel, then a professor at Stanford
University. In these studies, a child was
offered a choice between one small reward provided immediately or two small
rewards if they waited for a short period, approximately 15 minutes, during
which the tester left the room and then returned. (The reward was sometimes a marshmallow, but often a cookie or a pretzel.) In follow-up studies, the researchers found that
children who were able to wait longer for the preferred rewards tended to have
better life outcomes, as measured by SAT scores, educational attainment, body
mass index (BMI), and other life measures.
The experiment has its roots in an earlier one performed in Trinidad,
where Mischel noticed that the different ethnic groups living on the island had
contrasting stereotypes about
one another, in terms of the other's perceived recklessness, self-control, and ability to have fun. This study focused on
male and female children aged seven to nine (35 Black and 18 East Indian) in a rural Trinidad school.
The children were required to indicate a choice between receiving a 1¢ candy immediately, or having a 10¢ candy given to them in one week's time. Mischel reported a significant ethnic difference, with Indian children showing far more ability to delay gratification as compared to African students, as well as large age differences, and that "Comparison of the 'high' versus 'low' socioeconomic groups on the experimental choice did not yield a significant difference" Absence of the father was prevalent in the African-descent group but occurred only once in the East Indian group, and this variable showed the strongest link to delay of gratification, with children from intact families showing superior ability to delay. The first "Marshmallow Test" was a study conducted by Walter Mischel and Ebbe B. Ebbesen at Stanford University in 1960.
The children were required to indicate a choice between receiving a 1¢ candy immediately, or having a 10¢ candy given to them in one week's time. Mischel reported a significant ethnic difference, with Indian children showing far more ability to delay gratification as compared to African students, as well as large age differences, and that "Comparison of the 'high' versus 'low' socioeconomic groups on the experimental choice did not yield a significant difference" Absence of the father was prevalent in the African-descent group but occurred only once in the East Indian group, and this variable showed the strongest link to delay of gratification, with children from intact families showing superior ability to delay. The first "Marshmallow Test" was a study conducted by Walter Mischel and Ebbe B. Ebbesen at Stanford University in 1960.
The purpose
of the original study was to understand when the control of delayed
gratification, the ability to wait to obtain something that one wants, develops
in children. The original experiment took place at the Bing Nursery School
located at Stanford University, using children of ages four to six as
subjects. The children were led into a room, empty of distractions, where a
treat of their choice (Oreo cookie, marshmallow, or pretzel stick) was
placed on a table.
Children could eat the treat, the researchers
said, but if they waited for fifteen minutes without giving in to the
temptation, they would be rewarded with a second treat. Mischel observed
that some would "cover their eyes with their hands or turn around so that
they can't see the tray, others start kicking the desk, or tug on their pigtails,
or stroke the marshmallow as if it were a tiny stuffed animal," while
others would simply eat the marshmallow as soon as the researchers left.
In over 600 children who took
part in the experiment, a minority ate the marshmallow immediately. Of those
who attempted to delay, one third deferred gratification long enough to get the
second marshmallow. Age was a major determinant of deferred gratification.
On the table
in the experimental room there were five pretzels and an opaque cake tin.
Under the cake tin were five pretzels and two animal cookies. There were two
chairs in front of the table; on one chair was an empty cardboard box.
On the floor near the chair with the cardboard box on it were four battery
operated toys.
The experimenter pointed out the four toys; before the child could play with the toys, the experimenter asked the child to sit in the chair and then demonstrated each toy briefly and in a friendly manner, saying that they would play with the toys later on. Then the experimenter placed each toy in the cardboard box and out of sight of the child.
The experimenter explained to the child that the experimenter sometimes has to go out of the room but if the child eats a pretzel the experimenter will come back into the room. These instructions were repeated until the child seemed to understand them completely. The experimenter left the room and waited for the child to eat a pretzel – they did this four times.
The experimenter pointed out the four toys; before the child could play with the toys, the experimenter asked the child to sit in the chair and then demonstrated each toy briefly and in a friendly manner, saying that they would play with the toys later on. Then the experimenter placed each toy in the cardboard box and out of sight of the child.
The experimenter explained to the child that the experimenter sometimes has to go out of the room but if the child eats a pretzel the experimenter will come back into the room. These instructions were repeated until the child seemed to understand them completely. The experimenter left the room and waited for the child to eat a pretzel – they did this four times.
Next the experimenter opened
the cake tin to reveal two sets of reward objects to the child: five pretzels
and two animal crackers. The experimenter asked which of the two the child
liked better, and after the child chose, the experimenter explained that the
child could either continue waiting for the more preferred reward until the
experimenter returned, or the child could stop waiting by bringing the
experimenter back. If the child stopped waiting, then the child would receive
the less favoured reward and forgo the more preferred one.
Depending on the condition
and the child’s choice of preferred reward, the experimenter picked up the cake
tin and along with it either nothing, one of the rewards, or both. The
experimenter returned either as soon as the child signalled him to do so or
after 15 minutes.
In follow-up
studies, Mischel found unexpected correlations between the results of the
marshmallow test and the success of the children many years later.
The first
follow-up study, in 1988, showed that "preschool children who delayed
gratification longer in the self-imposed delay paradigm, were described more
than 10 years later by their parents as adolescents who were significantly more
competent.
A second follow-up study, in
1990, showed that the ability to delay gratification also correlated with
higher educational scores.
(Yes, not in any way my own work but an interesting case study inspired by a radio broadcast on BBC 4 Extra during last week)
No comments:
Post a Comment